Risk Comparisons, Conflict, and Risk Acceptability Claims
- 18 February 2004
- journal article
- Published by Wiley in Risk Analysis
- Vol. 24 (1), 131-145
- https://doi.org/10.1111/j.0272-4332.2004.00417.x
Abstract
Despite many claims for and against the use of risk comparisons in risk communication, few empirical studies have explored their effect. Even fewer have examined the public's relative preferences among different kinds of risk comparisons. Two studies, published in this journal in 1990 and 2003, used seven measures of "acceptability" to examine public reaction to 14 examples of risk comparisons, as used by a hypothetical factory manager to explain risks of his ethylene oxide plant. This study examined the effect on preferences of scenarios involving low or high conflict between the factory manager and residents of the hypothetical town (as had the 2003 study), and inclusion of a claim that the comparison demonstrated the risks' acceptability. It also tested the Finucane et al. (2000) affect hypothesis that information emphasizing low risks-as in these risk comparisons-would raise benefits estimates without changing risk estimates. Using similar but revised scenarios, risk comparison examples (10 instead of 14), and evaluation measures, an opportunity sample of 303 New Jersey residents rated the comparisons, and the risks and benefits of the factory. On average, all comparisons received positive ratings on all evaluation measures in all conditions. Direct and indirect measures showed that the conflict manipulation worked; overall, No-Conflict and Conflict scenarios evoked scores that were not significantly different. The attachment to each risk comparison of a risk acceptability claim ("So our factory's risks should be acceptable to you.") did not worsen ratings relative to conditions lacking this claim. Readers who did or did not see this claim were equally likely to infer an attempt to persuade them to accept the risk from the comparison. As in the 2003 article, there was great individual variability in inferred rankings of the risk comparisons. However, exposure to the risk comparisons did not reduce risk estimates significantly (while raising benefit estimates), and Conflict-Claim respondents found the risk of the hypothetical factory less acceptable than No-Conflict respondents. Results suggest that neither risk comparisons nor risk acceptability claims are automatically anathema to audiences, but they may have tiny or unintended effects on audience judgments about risky situations.Keywords
This publication has 16 references indexed in Scilit:
- Rooms with a view: Informal settings for public dialogueSociety & Natural Resources, 1996
- A Psychological Study of the Inverse Relationship Between Perceived Risk and Perceived BenefitRisk Analysis, 1994
- The Risks of “Putting the Numbers in Context”: A Cautionary TaleRisk Analysis, 1994
- The Interplay of Science, Values, and Experiences Among Scientists Asked to Evaluate the Hazards of Dioxin, Radon, and Environmental Tobacco SmokeRisk Analysis, 1992
- Risk communication activities of state health agencies.American Journal of Public Health, 1991
- What Do We Know About Making Risk Comparisons?Risk Analysis, 1990
- What Should We Know About Making Risk Comparisons?1Risk Analysis, 1990
- Ethical Considerations in Risk Communication Practice and Research1Risk Analysis, 1990
- Radon Risk Information and Voluntary Protection: Evidence from a Natural ExperimentRisk Analysis, 1987
- A Catalog of RisksHealth Physics, 1979