Trajectories of language delay from age 3 to 5: persistence, recovery and late onset
- 31 December 2013
- journal article
- Published by Wiley in International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders
- Vol. 49 (3), 304-316
- https://doi.org/10.1111/1460-6984.12073
Abstract
Background: Knowledge is scarce on what contributes to whether children with early language delay (LD) show persistent, recovering or sometimes late‐onset LD without a prior history of early LD in subsequent preschool years.Aims: To explore whether an integrative model of vital risk factors, including poor early communication skills, family history of language‐related difficulties and male gender, predicts the development of persistent, recovering or late‐onset LD trajectories from 3 to 5 years quantitatively and qualitatively differently.Methods & Procedures: LD was assessed by maternal reports on the Ages and Stages Questionnaire at 3 and 5 years for 10 587 children in The Norwegian Mother and Child Cohort Study. Children were classified across time as having no, late onset, transient or persistent LD. Multinomial logistic regression analyses included the integrative model of vital risk factors and covariates.Outcome & Results: Across time, 3%, 5% and 6.5% of the children displayed persistent, transient and late‐onset LD, respectively. The odds for persistent LD were doubled for boys and children with low language comprehension at 1.5 years; and tripled by late‐talking familial risk. These same odds decreased for transient LD, and even further for late‐onset LD. Familial risk for writing and reading difficulties especially increased the odds for late‐onset and persistent LD, while familial risk of unintelligible speech increased the odds for transient LD. Although girls had on average far better language comprehension than boys, low language comprehension was a stronger risk factor for persistent LD in girls.Conclusions & Implications: Preschool LD trajectories were uniquely predicted from the integrative risk model of poor early communicative skills, family history and male gender. This might benefit identification of different LD trajectories by supporting broader severe vulnerability for persistent LD, milder vulnerability for transient LD, and possibly a specific risk for reading and learning difficulties for children with late‐onset LD.Keywords
This publication has 26 references indexed in Scilit:
- Socio‐economic status and language acquisition: children's performance on the new Reynell Developmental Language ScalesInternational Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 2013
- Parental phonological memory contributes to prediction of outcome of late talkers from 20 months to 4 years: a longitudinal study of precursors of specific language impairmentJournal of Neurodevelopmental Disorders, 2012
- Relationships between language impairment, temperament, behavioural adjustment and maternal factors in a community sample of preschool childrenInternational Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 2011
- Predicting Language Outcomes at 4 Years of Age: Findings From Early Language in Victoria StudyPublished by American Academy of Pediatrics (AAP) ,2010
- Gender ratios for reading difficultiesDyslexia, 2009
- Late Language Emergence at 24 Months: An Epidemiological Study of Prevalence, Predictors, and CovariatesJournal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 2007
- Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: Conventional criteria versus new alternativesStructural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal, 1999
- Continuity of language abilities: An exploratory study of late‐ and early‐talking toddlersDevelopmental Neuropsychology, 1997
- Patterns Of Development in Late Talkers: Preschool YearsJournal of Childhool Communication Disorders, 1993
- Very Early Language Deficits in Dyslexic ChildrenChild Development, 1990