Communicating accuracy of tests to general practitioners: a controlled study
- 6 April 2002
- Vol. 324 (7341), 824-826
- https://doi.org/10.1136/bmj.324.7341.824
Abstract
Objective: To assess the extent to which different forms of summarising diagnostic test information influence general practitioners' ability to estimate disease probabilities. Design: Controlled questionnaire study. Setting: Three Swiss conferences in continuous medical education. Participants: 263 general practitioners. Intervention: Questionnaire with multiple choice questions about terms of test accuracy and a clinical vignette with the results of a diagnostic test described in three different ways (test result only, test result plus test sensitivity and specificity, test result plus the positive likelihood ratio presented in plain language). Main outcome measures: Doctors' knowledge and application of terms of test accuracy and estimation of disease probability in the clinical vignette. Results: The correct definitions for sensitivity and predictive value were chosen by 76% and 61% of the doctors respectively, but only 22% chose the correct answer for the post-test probability of a positive screening test. In the clinical vignette doctors given the test result only overestimated its diagnostic value (median attributed likelihood ratio (aLR)=9.0, against 2.54 reported in the literature). Providing the scan's sensitivity and specificity reduced the overestimation (median aLR=6.0) but to a lesser extent than simple wording of the likelihood ratio (median aLR=3.0). Conclusion: Most general practitioners recognised the correct definitions for sensitivity and positive predictive value but did not apply them correctly. Conveying test accuracy information in simple, non-technical language improved their ability to estimate disease probabilities accurately. What is already known on this topic Many doctors confuse the sensitivity of clinical tests and their positive predictive value Doctors tend to overestimate information derived from such tests and underestimate information from a patient's clinical history Most primary research on diagnostic accuracy is reported using sensitivity and specificity or likelihood ratios What this study adds In a cohort of experienced Swiss general practitioners most were unable to interpret correctly numerical information on the diagnostic accuracy of a screening test When presented with a positive result alone they grossly overestimated its value Adding information on the test's sensitivity and specificity moderated these overestimates, and expressing the same numerical information as a positive likelihood ratio in simple, non-technical language brought the estimates still closer to their true valuesKeywords
This publication has 11 references indexed in Scilit:
- Evaluation of outpatient hysteroscopy and ultrasonography in the diagnosis of endometrial diseasePublished by Wolters Kluwer Health ,2002
- Systematic reviews in health care: Systematic reviews of evaluations of diagnostic and screening testsBMJ, 2001
- Communicating Statistical InformationScience, 2000
- Why we need large, simple studies of the clinical examination: the problem and a proposed solutionThe Lancet, 1999
- Evaluation of Diagnostic Imaging Tests: Diagnostic Probability EstimationJournal of Clinical Epidemiology, 1998
- Do We Know What Inappropriate Laboratory Utilization Is?JAMA, 1998
- Academic Calculations versus Clinical Judgments: Practicing Physicians’ Use of Quantitative Measures of Test AccuracyAmerican Journal Of Medicine, 1998
- Computer-based Models to Identify High-risk Children with AsthmaAmerican Journal of Respiratory and Critical Care Medicine, 1998
- A study to develop clinical decision rules for the use of radiography in acute ankle injuriesAnnals of Emergency Medicine, 1992
- Probabilistic reasoning in clinical medicine: Problems and opportunitiesPublished by Cambridge University Press (CUP) ,1982