The process of feature binding
Open Access
- 1 January 2013
- journal article
- editorial
- Published by Frontiers Media SA in Frontiers in Psychology
- Vol. 4, 207
- https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00207
Abstract
The integration of different stimulus properties as an object is the process of feature binding. The research topic “Inhibition in the process of feature binding” aimed to debate which features bind together, why, and how; and what happens to features which are not integrated. The authors of the review articles took up the debate in earnest. Wyatte et al. (2012) emphasize that the process of binding does not require any special neural substrates. The two mechanisms of inhibition and top-down feedback are sufficient to explain the process of object recognition. Their computational model describes how these established principles of neural processing work over time to solve the binding problem. Particularly critical of the idea of neural synchrony, often held forth as “the” mechanism underlying feature binding, they contend that it helps only to create a contrast between relevant and irrelevant features. It is top-down feedback, which reinforces relevant information. The relevance of features is the key factor in the process of binding for Jaswal (2012). She attempts an integration of physiological and psychological literature, highlighting that only relevant features bind together in a rather slow process, which concomitantly ensures that the irrelevant features are actively inhibited. In a more expanded overview, Velik (2012) reaches similar conclusions—that irrelevant information is deleted from the system in the process of binding. However, whilst Jaswal (2012) bases her conclusions on empirical studies, Velik (2012) presents a computational model in which filter mechanisms work to suppress irrelevant information. Krummenacher and Müller (2012) review evidence from behavioral and ERP studies to confirm that separate feature dimensions are important in the pre-selective phase of feature binding. However, once the bound object emerges, feature based effects are not as dominant. This corroborates with the aforementioned views that only relevant features are carried forward. Herzog et al. (2012) also address how we process features prior to and during the process of binding. Using the sequential metacontrast paradigm, they explore binding as a sub-process in the Gestalt experience of grouping elements into wholes. They contend that computation of features is in itself a process and not an instantaneous event. Stimulus representations are dynamic and binding probably occurs simultaneously with the processing of independent features. Meier and Rey-Mermet (2012) discuss episodic context binding using bivalent stimuli. The ambiguity of bivalent stimuli is itself a “feature” that enters binding and influences subsequent behavior in consonance with the bivalency effect. It is interesting that Meier and Ray-Mermet propose that this conflict is not relevant to the task. In fact, this ambiguity and concomitant conflict is a cardinal feature of bivalent stimuli, which reactivates whenever the context is redintegrated. Moving from features to objects, Dent et al. (2012) review behavioral and physiological evidence for the process of distracter inhibition in visual search. They postulate that it is a resource demanding active process, which is parallel in nature, such that all distracters are deleted, and the target alone remains as the item to be processed further. The four articles contributing original research extend the study of binding to novel avenues. As merits empirical investigations, all studies not only document details of current work in these new areas, they also indicate hypotheses for future investigations. Delogu et al. (2012) enter the arena of audition to investigate the link between location and temporal order. The recall of temporal order is weakened more than locations, whilst binding in visual as well as auditory domains. This suggests that location is encoded relatively automatically during binding, but recall of temporal order is resource demanding. Similar results across visual and auditory domains suggest that binding of location and temporal order may involve shared, modality-independent physiological mechanisms. This special link between “when” and “where” features, merits future explorations. Giersch et al. (2012) scale up from features to explore grouping and regrouping between objects among schizophrenics and healthy controls. Patients were particularly slow to detect connected targets when the attentional focus was on unconnected pairs. This effect was found only for targets presented within the same hemifield. Thus, schizophrenics do not regroup stimuli in the same way as healthy controls. Speculations can be made regarding the role of connectivity between hemispheres in grouping. Further, grouping and regrouping are different (maybe parallel) mechanisms, the former relying on automatic processing, whilst the latter demand attentional resources. The two original research articles based on computational models conceptualize binding as part of other processes. Schrobsdorff et al. (2012) propose that binding is an essential phase in inhibition. In their model of the negative priming effect, features are activated, bound into object entities, and related to their context. Inhibitory processes and changing thresholds implement the concept of selective attention. Thus, binding becomes a sub-process in a general model of inhibition. Davelaar (2013) evokes the concept of binding to explain the Gratton effect in the Eriksen flanker task. The Gratton effect is the reduction in the interference due to flankers after incongruent, as compared with congruent trials. Results of experiments that separate the contributions from target, flanker, and response repetition, show that flanker repetition alone is sufficient to produce congruency effects in sequences of trials. He postulates that representations of targets, flankers, and response(s) are associated in a task set. This work is an excellent example of...Keywords
This publication has 61 references indexed in Scilit:
- Relevance and application of sortase and sortase-dependent proteins in lactic acid bacteriaFrontiers in Microbiology, 2013
- Influence of Exposure Time on Gene Expression by Human Intestinal Epithelial Cells Exposed to Lactobacillus acidophilusApplied and Environmental Microbiology, 2012
- Identification of a high-molecular-mass Lactobacillus epithelium adhesin (LEA) of Lactobacillus crispatus ST1 that binds to stratified squamous epitheliumMicrobiology, 2012
- Sortase enzymes in Gram‐positive bacteriaMolecular Microbiology, 2011
- Cell Wall Anchoring of the 37-Kilodalton Oncofetal Antigen by Lactobacillus plantarum for Mucosal Cancer Vaccine DeliveryApplied and Environmental Microbiology, 2010
- Functionality of Sortase A in Lactococcus lactisApplied and Environmental Microbiology, 2010
- Comparative genomic analysis of Lactobacillus rhamnosus GG reveals pili containing a human- mucus binding proteinProceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 2009
- Comparative genomics of the lactic acid bacteriaProceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 2006
- Multireplicon genome architecture of Lactobacillus salivariusProceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 2006
- Complete sequence and comparative genome analysis of the dairy bacterium Streptococcus thermophilusNature Biotechnology, 2004